Home > Research > Publications & Outputs > Inferential revision in narrative texts

Electronic data

  • 2015_PerezCainCastellanosBajo

    Rights statement: The final publication is available at Springer via http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-015-0528-0

    Accepted author manuscript, 411 KB, PDF document

    Available under license: CC BY-NC: Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License

Links

Text available via DOI:

View graph of relations

Inferential revision in narrative texts: an ERP study

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Published

Standard

Inferential revision in narrative texts: an ERP study. / Perez, Ana; Cain, Kate; Castellanos, Maria et al.
In: Memory and Cognition, Vol. 43, No. 8, 11.2015, p. 1105-1135.

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Harvard

Perez, A, Cain, K, Castellanos, M & Bajo, T 2015, 'Inferential revision in narrative texts: an ERP study', Memory and Cognition, vol. 43, no. 8, pp. 1105-1135. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-015-0528-0

APA

Perez, A., Cain, K., Castellanos, M., & Bajo, T. (2015). Inferential revision in narrative texts: an ERP study. Memory and Cognition, 43(8), 1105-1135. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-015-0528-0

Vancouver

Perez A, Cain K, Castellanos M, Bajo T. Inferential revision in narrative texts: an ERP study. Memory and Cognition. 2015 Nov;43(8):1105-1135. Epub 2015 Jun 6. doi: 10.3758/s13421-015-0528-0

Author

Perez, Ana ; Cain, Kate ; Castellanos, Maria et al. / Inferential revision in narrative texts : an ERP study. In: Memory and Cognition. 2015 ; Vol. 43, No. 8. pp. 1105-1135.

Bibtex

@article{1584ce7073514916943586fc83f79a94,
title = "Inferential revision in narrative texts: an ERP study",
abstract = "We evaluated the process of inferential revision during text comprehension in adults. Participants with high or low working memory read short texts, in which the introduction supported two plausible concepts (e.g., {\textquoteleft}guitar/violin{\textquoteright}), although one was more probable ({\textquoteleft}guitar{\textquoteright}). There were three possible continuations: a neutral sentence, which did not refer back to either concept; a no revise sentence, which referred to a general property consistent with either concept (e.g., {\textquoteleft}…beautiful curved body{\textquoteright}); and a revise sentence, which referred to a property that was consistent with only the less likely concept (e.g., {\textquoteleft}…matching bow{\textquoteright}). Readers took longer to read the sentence in the revise condition, indicating that they were able to evaluate their comprehension and detect a mismatch. In a final sentence, a target noun referred to the alternative concept supported in the revise condition (e.g., {\textquoteleft}violin{\textquoteright}). ERPs indicated that both working memory groups were able to evaluate their comprehension of the text (P3a), but only high working memory readers were able to revise their initial incorrect interpretation (P3b) and integrate the new information (N400) when reading the revise sentence. Low working memory readers had difficulties inhibiting the no longer relevant interpretation and thus failed to revise their situation model, and they experienced problems integrating semantically related information into an accurate memory representation.",
keywords = "Revising information, Inference making, Working memory",
author = "Ana Perez and Kate Cain and Maria Castellanos and Teresa Bajo",
note = "The final publication is available at Springer via http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-015-0528-0",
year = "2015",
month = nov,
doi = "10.3758/s13421-015-0528-0",
language = "English",
volume = "43",
pages = "1105--1135",
journal = "Memory and Cognition",
issn = "0090-502X",
publisher = "Springer New York",
number = "8",

}

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - Inferential revision in narrative texts

T2 - an ERP study

AU - Perez, Ana

AU - Cain, Kate

AU - Castellanos, Maria

AU - Bajo, Teresa

N1 - The final publication is available at Springer via http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-015-0528-0

PY - 2015/11

Y1 - 2015/11

N2 - We evaluated the process of inferential revision during text comprehension in adults. Participants with high or low working memory read short texts, in which the introduction supported two plausible concepts (e.g., ‘guitar/violin’), although one was more probable (‘guitar’). There were three possible continuations: a neutral sentence, which did not refer back to either concept; a no revise sentence, which referred to a general property consistent with either concept (e.g., ‘…beautiful curved body’); and a revise sentence, which referred to a property that was consistent with only the less likely concept (e.g., ‘…matching bow’). Readers took longer to read the sentence in the revise condition, indicating that they were able to evaluate their comprehension and detect a mismatch. In a final sentence, a target noun referred to the alternative concept supported in the revise condition (e.g., ‘violin’). ERPs indicated that both working memory groups were able to evaluate their comprehension of the text (P3a), but only high working memory readers were able to revise their initial incorrect interpretation (P3b) and integrate the new information (N400) when reading the revise sentence. Low working memory readers had difficulties inhibiting the no longer relevant interpretation and thus failed to revise their situation model, and they experienced problems integrating semantically related information into an accurate memory representation.

AB - We evaluated the process of inferential revision during text comprehension in adults. Participants with high or low working memory read short texts, in which the introduction supported two plausible concepts (e.g., ‘guitar/violin’), although one was more probable (‘guitar’). There were three possible continuations: a neutral sentence, which did not refer back to either concept; a no revise sentence, which referred to a general property consistent with either concept (e.g., ‘…beautiful curved body’); and a revise sentence, which referred to a property that was consistent with only the less likely concept (e.g., ‘…matching bow’). Readers took longer to read the sentence in the revise condition, indicating that they were able to evaluate their comprehension and detect a mismatch. In a final sentence, a target noun referred to the alternative concept supported in the revise condition (e.g., ‘violin’). ERPs indicated that both working memory groups were able to evaluate their comprehension of the text (P3a), but only high working memory readers were able to revise their initial incorrect interpretation (P3b) and integrate the new information (N400) when reading the revise sentence. Low working memory readers had difficulties inhibiting the no longer relevant interpretation and thus failed to revise their situation model, and they experienced problems integrating semantically related information into an accurate memory representation.

KW - Revising information

KW - Inference making

KW - Working memory

U2 - 10.3758/s13421-015-0528-0

DO - 10.3758/s13421-015-0528-0

M3 - Journal article

VL - 43

SP - 1105

EP - 1135

JO - Memory and Cognition

JF - Memory and Cognition

SN - 0090-502X

IS - 8

ER -