Home > Research > Publications & Outputs > Size-brightness correspondence
View graph of relations

Size-brightness correspondence: crosstalk and congruity among dimensions of connotative meaning

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Published

Standard

Size-brightness correspondence: crosstalk and congruity among dimensions of connotative meaning. / Walker, Peter; Walker, Laura.
In: Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, Vol. 74, No. 6, 2012, p. 1226-1240.

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Harvard

APA

Vancouver

Walker P, Walker L. Size-brightness correspondence: crosstalk and congruity among dimensions of connotative meaning. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics. 2012;74(6):1226-1240. Epub 2012 Apr 6. doi: 10.3758/s13414-012-0297-9

Author

Walker, Peter ; Walker, Laura. / Size-brightness correspondence : crosstalk and congruity among dimensions of connotative meaning. In: Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics. 2012 ; Vol. 74, No. 6. pp. 1226-1240.

Bibtex

@article{895674f02f8542cebfc074fdfdbcb855,
title = "Size-brightness correspondence: crosstalk and congruity among dimensions of connotative meaning",
abstract = "Following Karwoski, Odbert, and Osgood (Journal of General Psychology 26:199–222, 1942), in the present article, cross-sensory correspondences are proposed to reflect the alignment of, and extensive bidirectional cross-activation among, dimensions of connotative meaning. The size–brightness correspondence predicted on this basis (in which smaller is aligned with brighter) was confirmed in two ways. First, when participants explored three wooden balls of different size by touch alone and indicated how bright they thought each of them was, the smaller ball was judged to be brighter than the bigger ball. Second, when these two balls served as response keys in a speeded brightness-classification task, participants were quicker and more likely to be correct when confirming that a stimulus was bright (dark) when this required them to press the smaller (bigger) key, than when it required them to press the bigger (smaller) key. This congruity effect originated from interactions embedded in the later stages of information processing concerned with stimulus classification and response selection. These results, together with the observation that the cross-sensory features associated with smallness are the same as those associated with higher pitch sounds (i.e., both attributes are more active, brighter, faster, lighter in weight, quieter, sharper, and weaker than their opposites), support the suggestion that there exists a core set of cross-sensory correspondences that emerges whichever stimulus feature is used to probe it.",
keywords = "Size-brightness correspondence , Cross-sensory correspondences , Connotative meaning , Congruity , Speeded classification",
author = "Peter Walker and Laura Walker",
year = "2012",
doi = "10.3758/s13414-012-0297-9",
language = "English",
volume = "74",
pages = "1226--1240",
journal = "Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics",
issn = "1943-3921",
publisher = "Springer",
number = "6",

}

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - Size-brightness correspondence

T2 - crosstalk and congruity among dimensions of connotative meaning

AU - Walker, Peter

AU - Walker, Laura

PY - 2012

Y1 - 2012

N2 - Following Karwoski, Odbert, and Osgood (Journal of General Psychology 26:199–222, 1942), in the present article, cross-sensory correspondences are proposed to reflect the alignment of, and extensive bidirectional cross-activation among, dimensions of connotative meaning. The size–brightness correspondence predicted on this basis (in which smaller is aligned with brighter) was confirmed in two ways. First, when participants explored three wooden balls of different size by touch alone and indicated how bright they thought each of them was, the smaller ball was judged to be brighter than the bigger ball. Second, when these two balls served as response keys in a speeded brightness-classification task, participants were quicker and more likely to be correct when confirming that a stimulus was bright (dark) when this required them to press the smaller (bigger) key, than when it required them to press the bigger (smaller) key. This congruity effect originated from interactions embedded in the later stages of information processing concerned with stimulus classification and response selection. These results, together with the observation that the cross-sensory features associated with smallness are the same as those associated with higher pitch sounds (i.e., both attributes are more active, brighter, faster, lighter in weight, quieter, sharper, and weaker than their opposites), support the suggestion that there exists a core set of cross-sensory correspondences that emerges whichever stimulus feature is used to probe it.

AB - Following Karwoski, Odbert, and Osgood (Journal of General Psychology 26:199–222, 1942), in the present article, cross-sensory correspondences are proposed to reflect the alignment of, and extensive bidirectional cross-activation among, dimensions of connotative meaning. The size–brightness correspondence predicted on this basis (in which smaller is aligned with brighter) was confirmed in two ways. First, when participants explored three wooden balls of different size by touch alone and indicated how bright they thought each of them was, the smaller ball was judged to be brighter than the bigger ball. Second, when these two balls served as response keys in a speeded brightness-classification task, participants were quicker and more likely to be correct when confirming that a stimulus was bright (dark) when this required them to press the smaller (bigger) key, than when it required them to press the bigger (smaller) key. This congruity effect originated from interactions embedded in the later stages of information processing concerned with stimulus classification and response selection. These results, together with the observation that the cross-sensory features associated with smallness are the same as those associated with higher pitch sounds (i.e., both attributes are more active, brighter, faster, lighter in weight, quieter, sharper, and weaker than their opposites), support the suggestion that there exists a core set of cross-sensory correspondences that emerges whichever stimulus feature is used to probe it.

KW - Size-brightness correspondence

KW - Cross-sensory correspondences

KW - Connotative meaning

KW - Congruity

KW - Speeded classification

UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=84864548724&partnerID=8YFLogxK

U2 - 10.3758/s13414-012-0297-9

DO - 10.3758/s13414-012-0297-9

M3 - Journal article

AN - SCOPUS:84864548724

VL - 74

SP - 1226

EP - 1240

JO - Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics

JF - Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics

SN - 1943-3921

IS - 6

ER -