Home > Research > Publications & Outputs > Changing minds about minds

Links

Text available via DOI:

View graph of relations

Changing minds about minds: Evidence that people are too sceptical about animal sentience

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Published

Standard

Changing minds about minds: Evidence that people are too sceptical about animal sentience. / Leach, Stefan; Sutton, Robbie M.; Dhont, Kristof et al.
In: Cognition, Vol. 230, 105263, 31.01.2023.

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Harvard

APA

Leach, S., Sutton, R. M., Dhont, K., Douglas, K. M., & Bergström, Z. M. (2023). Changing minds about minds: Evidence that people are too sceptical about animal sentience. Cognition, 230, Article 105263. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105263

Vancouver

Leach S, Sutton RM, Dhont K, Douglas KM, Bergström ZM. Changing minds about minds: Evidence that people are too sceptical about animal sentience. Cognition. 2023 Jan 31;230:105263. Epub 2022 Sept 12. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105263

Author

Leach, Stefan ; Sutton, Robbie M. ; Dhont, Kristof et al. / Changing minds about minds : Evidence that people are too sceptical about animal sentience. In: Cognition. 2023 ; Vol. 230.

Bibtex

@article{6645ed196f294356ba42ac224b584ff9,
title = "Changing minds about minds: Evidence that people are too sceptical about animal sentience",
abstract = "Our relationships with other animals are governed by how we view their capacity for sentience and suffering. However, there is currently little agreement as to whether people's beliefs about animal minds are largely accurate or inaccurate. We used an innovative task to examine how people update their beliefs in response to noisy but informative clues about animal minds. This allowed us to compare participants' posterior beliefs to what a normative participant ought to believe if they conform to Bayes' theorem. Five studies (four pre-registered; n = 2417) found that participants shifted their beliefs too far in response to clues that suggested animals do not have minds (i.e., overshooting what a normative participant ought to believe), but not far enough in response to clues that suggested animals have minds (i.e., falling short of what a normative participant ought to believe). A final study demonstrated that this effect was attenuated when humans were the targets of belief. The findings demonstrate that people underestimate animal minds in a way that can be said to be inaccurate and highlight the role of belief updating in downplaying evidence of animal minds. The findings are discussed in relation to speciesist beliefs about the supremacy of humans over animals.",
author = "Stefan Leach and Sutton, {Robbie M.} and Kristof Dhont and Douglas, {Karen M.} and Bergstr{\"o}m, {Zara M.}",
year = "2023",
month = jan,
day = "31",
doi = "10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105263",
language = "English",
volume = "230",
journal = "Cognition",
issn = "0010-0277",
publisher = "Elsevier",

}

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - Changing minds about minds

T2 - Evidence that people are too sceptical about animal sentience

AU - Leach, Stefan

AU - Sutton, Robbie M.

AU - Dhont, Kristof

AU - Douglas, Karen M.

AU - Bergström, Zara M.

PY - 2023/1/31

Y1 - 2023/1/31

N2 - Our relationships with other animals are governed by how we view their capacity for sentience and suffering. However, there is currently little agreement as to whether people's beliefs about animal minds are largely accurate or inaccurate. We used an innovative task to examine how people update their beliefs in response to noisy but informative clues about animal minds. This allowed us to compare participants' posterior beliefs to what a normative participant ought to believe if they conform to Bayes' theorem. Five studies (four pre-registered; n = 2417) found that participants shifted their beliefs too far in response to clues that suggested animals do not have minds (i.e., overshooting what a normative participant ought to believe), but not far enough in response to clues that suggested animals have minds (i.e., falling short of what a normative participant ought to believe). A final study demonstrated that this effect was attenuated when humans were the targets of belief. The findings demonstrate that people underestimate animal minds in a way that can be said to be inaccurate and highlight the role of belief updating in downplaying evidence of animal minds. The findings are discussed in relation to speciesist beliefs about the supremacy of humans over animals.

AB - Our relationships with other animals are governed by how we view their capacity for sentience and suffering. However, there is currently little agreement as to whether people's beliefs about animal minds are largely accurate or inaccurate. We used an innovative task to examine how people update their beliefs in response to noisy but informative clues about animal minds. This allowed us to compare participants' posterior beliefs to what a normative participant ought to believe if they conform to Bayes' theorem. Five studies (four pre-registered; n = 2417) found that participants shifted their beliefs too far in response to clues that suggested animals do not have minds (i.e., overshooting what a normative participant ought to believe), but not far enough in response to clues that suggested animals have minds (i.e., falling short of what a normative participant ought to believe). A final study demonstrated that this effect was attenuated when humans were the targets of belief. The findings demonstrate that people underestimate animal minds in a way that can be said to be inaccurate and highlight the role of belief updating in downplaying evidence of animal minds. The findings are discussed in relation to speciesist beliefs about the supremacy of humans over animals.

U2 - 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105263

DO - 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105263

M3 - Journal article

VL - 230

JO - Cognition

JF - Cognition

SN - 0010-0277

M1 - 105263

ER -