Home > Research > Publications & Outputs > Dual-goal facilitation in Wason's 2-4-6 task: W...
View graph of relations

Dual-goal facilitation in Wason's 2-4-6 task: What mediates successful rule discovery?

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Published

Standard

Dual-goal facilitation in Wason's 2-4-6 task: What mediates successful rule discovery? / Ball, L. J.; Gale, M.
In: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , Vol. 59, No. 5, 05.2006, p. 873-885.

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Harvard

Ball, LJ & Gale, M 2006, 'Dual-goal facilitation in Wason's 2-4-6 task: What mediates successful rule discovery?', The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , vol. 59, no. 5, pp. 873-885. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980543000051

APA

Vancouver

Ball LJ, Gale M. Dual-goal facilitation in Wason's 2-4-6 task: What mediates successful rule discovery? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology . 2006 May;59(5):873-885. doi: 10.1080/02724980543000051

Author

Ball, L. J. ; Gale, M. / Dual-goal facilitation in Wason's 2-4-6 task: What mediates successful rule discovery?. In: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology . 2006 ; Vol. 59, No. 5. pp. 873-885.

Bibtex

@article{43ae16bec08a4598a437de85f6077b4f,
title = "Dual-goal facilitation in Wason's 2-4-6 task: What mediates successful rule discovery?",
abstract = "The standard 2-4-6 task requires discovery of a single rule and produces success rates of about 20%, whereas the dual-goal (DG) version requests discovery of two complementary rules and elevates success to over 60%. The experiment examined two explanations of DG superiority: Evans' (1989) positivity-bias account, and Wharton, Cheng, and Wickens' (1993) goal-complementarity theory. Two DG conditions were employed that varied the linguistic labelling of rules (either positively labelled Dax vs. Med, or mixed-valence {"}fits{"} vs. {"}does not fit{"}). Solution-success results supported the goal-complementarity theory since facilitation arose in both DG conditions relative to single-goal tasks, irrespective of the linguistic labelling of hypotheses. DG instructions also altered quantitative and qualitative aspects of hypothesis-testing behaviour, and analyses revealed the novel result that the production of at least a single descending triple mediates between DG instructions and task success. We propose that the identification of an appropriate contrast class that delimits the scope of complementary rules may be facilitated through the generation of a descending instance. Overall, our findings can best be accommodated by Oaksford and Chater's (1994) iterative counterfactual model of hypotheses testing, which can readily subsume key elements of the goal-complementarity theory. A subset of the data reported here formed part of a poster presented at the Twenty-Fourth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Fairfax, Virginia, USA, August 2002.",
author = "Ball, {L. J.} and M. Gale",
note = "Ball was lead author, designed experiments, co-wrote manuscript. Ball was co-author's PhD supervisor. RAE_import_type : Journal article RAE_uoa_type : Psychology",
year = "2006",
month = may,
doi = "10.1080/02724980543000051",
language = "English",
volume = "59",
pages = "873--885",
journal = "The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology ",
issn = "1747-0218",
publisher = "Psychology Press Ltd",
number = "5",

}

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - Dual-goal facilitation in Wason's 2-4-6 task: What mediates successful rule discovery?

AU - Ball, L. J.

AU - Gale, M.

N1 - Ball was lead author, designed experiments, co-wrote manuscript. Ball was co-author's PhD supervisor. RAE_import_type : Journal article RAE_uoa_type : Psychology

PY - 2006/5

Y1 - 2006/5

N2 - The standard 2-4-6 task requires discovery of a single rule and produces success rates of about 20%, whereas the dual-goal (DG) version requests discovery of two complementary rules and elevates success to over 60%. The experiment examined two explanations of DG superiority: Evans' (1989) positivity-bias account, and Wharton, Cheng, and Wickens' (1993) goal-complementarity theory. Two DG conditions were employed that varied the linguistic labelling of rules (either positively labelled Dax vs. Med, or mixed-valence "fits" vs. "does not fit"). Solution-success results supported the goal-complementarity theory since facilitation arose in both DG conditions relative to single-goal tasks, irrespective of the linguistic labelling of hypotheses. DG instructions also altered quantitative and qualitative aspects of hypothesis-testing behaviour, and analyses revealed the novel result that the production of at least a single descending triple mediates between DG instructions and task success. We propose that the identification of an appropriate contrast class that delimits the scope of complementary rules may be facilitated through the generation of a descending instance. Overall, our findings can best be accommodated by Oaksford and Chater's (1994) iterative counterfactual model of hypotheses testing, which can readily subsume key elements of the goal-complementarity theory. A subset of the data reported here formed part of a poster presented at the Twenty-Fourth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Fairfax, Virginia, USA, August 2002.

AB - The standard 2-4-6 task requires discovery of a single rule and produces success rates of about 20%, whereas the dual-goal (DG) version requests discovery of two complementary rules and elevates success to over 60%. The experiment examined two explanations of DG superiority: Evans' (1989) positivity-bias account, and Wharton, Cheng, and Wickens' (1993) goal-complementarity theory. Two DG conditions were employed that varied the linguistic labelling of rules (either positively labelled Dax vs. Med, or mixed-valence "fits" vs. "does not fit"). Solution-success results supported the goal-complementarity theory since facilitation arose in both DG conditions relative to single-goal tasks, irrespective of the linguistic labelling of hypotheses. DG instructions also altered quantitative and qualitative aspects of hypothesis-testing behaviour, and analyses revealed the novel result that the production of at least a single descending triple mediates between DG instructions and task success. We propose that the identification of an appropriate contrast class that delimits the scope of complementary rules may be facilitated through the generation of a descending instance. Overall, our findings can best be accommodated by Oaksford and Chater's (1994) iterative counterfactual model of hypotheses testing, which can readily subsume key elements of the goal-complementarity theory. A subset of the data reported here formed part of a poster presented at the Twenty-Fourth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Fairfax, Virginia, USA, August 2002.

U2 - 10.1080/02724980543000051

DO - 10.1080/02724980543000051

M3 - Journal article

VL - 59

SP - 873

EP - 885

JO - The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

JF - The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

SN - 1747-0218

IS - 5

ER -