Home > Research > Publications & Outputs > Examining the contribution of markedness to the...

Links

Text available via DOI:

View graph of relations

Examining the contribution of markedness to the L2 processing of spanish person agreement

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Published

Standard

Examining the contribution of markedness to the L2 processing of spanish person agreement. / Bañón, José Alemán; Miller, David; Rothman, Jason.
In: Studies in Second Language Acquisition, Vol. 43, No. 4, 10.09.2021, p. 699-728.

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Harvard

Bañón, JA, Miller, D & Rothman, J 2021, 'Examining the contribution of markedness to the L2 processing of spanish person agreement', Studies in Second Language Acquisition, vol. 43, no. 4, pp. 699-728. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263120000479

APA

Vancouver

Bañón JA, Miller D, Rothman J. Examining the contribution of markedness to the L2 processing of spanish person agreement. Studies in Second Language Acquisition. 2021 Sept 10;43(4):699-728. Epub 2020 Nov 10. doi: 10.1017/S0272263120000479

Author

Bañón, José Alemán ; Miller, David ; Rothman, Jason. / Examining the contribution of markedness to the L2 processing of spanish person agreement. In: Studies in Second Language Acquisition. 2021 ; Vol. 43, No. 4. pp. 699-728.

Bibtex

@article{48423a4ea719491199a61648206ecbff,
title = "Examining the contribution of markedness to the L2 processing of spanish person agreement",
abstract = "We used event-related potentials to investigate how markedness impacts person agreement in English-speaking learners of L2-Spanish. Markedness was examined by probing agreement with both first-person (marked) and third-person (unmarked) subjects. Agreement was manipulated by crossing first-person subjects with third-person verbs and vice versa. Native speakers showed a P600 for both errors, larger for first-person subject + third-person verb violations. This aligns with claims that, when the first element in the dependency is marked (first person), the parser generates stronger predictions regarding upcoming agreeing elements using feature activation. Twenty-two upper-intermediate/advanced learners elicited a P600 across both errors. Learners were equally accurate detecting both errors, but the P600 was marginally reduced for first-person subject + third-person verb violations, suggesting that learners overused unmarked forms (third person) online. However, this asymmetry mainly characterized lower-proficiency learners. Results suggest that markedness impacts L2 agreement without constraining it, although learners are less likely to use marked features top-down.",
author = "Ba{\~n}{\'o}n, {Jos{\'e} Alem{\'a}n} and David Miller and Jason Rothman",
note = "Publisher Copyright: {\textcopyright}",
year = "2021",
month = sep,
day = "10",
doi = "10.1017/S0272263120000479",
language = "English",
volume = "43",
pages = "699--728",
journal = "Studies in Second Language Acquisition",
issn = "0272-2631",
publisher = "Cambridge University Press",
number = "4",

}

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - Examining the contribution of markedness to the L2 processing of spanish person agreement

AU - Bañón, José Alemán

AU - Miller, David

AU - Rothman, Jason

N1 - Publisher Copyright: ©

PY - 2021/9/10

Y1 - 2021/9/10

N2 - We used event-related potentials to investigate how markedness impacts person agreement in English-speaking learners of L2-Spanish. Markedness was examined by probing agreement with both first-person (marked) and third-person (unmarked) subjects. Agreement was manipulated by crossing first-person subjects with third-person verbs and vice versa. Native speakers showed a P600 for both errors, larger for first-person subject + third-person verb violations. This aligns with claims that, when the first element in the dependency is marked (first person), the parser generates stronger predictions regarding upcoming agreeing elements using feature activation. Twenty-two upper-intermediate/advanced learners elicited a P600 across both errors. Learners were equally accurate detecting both errors, but the P600 was marginally reduced for first-person subject + third-person verb violations, suggesting that learners overused unmarked forms (third person) online. However, this asymmetry mainly characterized lower-proficiency learners. Results suggest that markedness impacts L2 agreement without constraining it, although learners are less likely to use marked features top-down.

AB - We used event-related potentials to investigate how markedness impacts person agreement in English-speaking learners of L2-Spanish. Markedness was examined by probing agreement with both first-person (marked) and third-person (unmarked) subjects. Agreement was manipulated by crossing first-person subjects with third-person verbs and vice versa. Native speakers showed a P600 for both errors, larger for first-person subject + third-person verb violations. This aligns with claims that, when the first element in the dependency is marked (first person), the parser generates stronger predictions regarding upcoming agreeing elements using feature activation. Twenty-two upper-intermediate/advanced learners elicited a P600 across both errors. Learners were equally accurate detecting both errors, but the P600 was marginally reduced for first-person subject + third-person verb violations, suggesting that learners overused unmarked forms (third person) online. However, this asymmetry mainly characterized lower-proficiency learners. Results suggest that markedness impacts L2 agreement without constraining it, although learners are less likely to use marked features top-down.

U2 - 10.1017/S0272263120000479

DO - 10.1017/S0272263120000479

M3 - Journal article

AN - SCOPUS:85095980124

VL - 43

SP - 699

EP - 728

JO - Studies in Second Language Acquisition

JF - Studies in Second Language Acquisition

SN - 0272-2631

IS - 4

ER -