Home > Research > Publications & Outputs > Flexible use of mutual exclusivity in word lear...

Associated organisational unit

Links

Text available via DOI:

View graph of relations

Flexible use of mutual exclusivity in word learning

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Published

Standard

Flexible use of mutual exclusivity in word learning. / Kalashnikova, Marina; Mattock, Karen Janette; Monaghan, Padraic John.
In: Language Learning and Development, Vol. 12, No. 1, 2016, p. 79-91.

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Harvard

Kalashnikova, M, Mattock, KJ & Monaghan, PJ 2016, 'Flexible use of mutual exclusivity in word learning', Language Learning and Development, vol. 12, no. 1, pp. 79-91. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2015.1023443

APA

Vancouver

Kalashnikova M, Mattock KJ, Monaghan PJ. Flexible use of mutual exclusivity in word learning. Language Learning and Development. 2016;12(1):79-91. Epub 2015 Nov 9. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2015.1023443

Author

Kalashnikova, Marina ; Mattock, Karen Janette ; Monaghan, Padraic John. / Flexible use of mutual exclusivity in word learning. In: Language Learning and Development. 2016 ; Vol. 12, No. 1. pp. 79-91.

Bibtex

@article{00e0339c85e646869e426ea24391b3cf,
title = "Flexible use of mutual exclusivity in word learning",
abstract = "From an early age, children apply the mutual exclusivity (ME) assumption, demonstrating preference for one-to-one mappings between words and their referents. However, for the acquisition of referentially overlapping terms, ME use must be suspended. We test whether contextual cues to intended meaning, in the form of presence of a speaker, may be critical for flexible ME application. Four- to five-year-old children were tested on two word learning tasks requiring flexible use of ME, respectively. In Experiment 1, children saw video recordings of the speakers introducing the novel labels. All children successfully applied ME and succeeded in accepting lexical overlap. In Experiment 2, with audio recordings of speakers only, children were unsuccessful at accepting lexical overlap. Thus, flexible use of ME relies on a developing ability to utilise the contextual information present in communicative interactions.",
author = "Marina Kalashnikova and Mattock, {Karen Janette} and Monaghan, {Padraic John}",
year = "2016",
doi = "10.1080/15475441.2015.1023443",
language = "English",
volume = "12",
pages = "79--91",
journal = "Language Learning and Development",
issn = "1547-5441",
publisher = "Psychology Press Ltd",
number = "1",

}

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - Flexible use of mutual exclusivity in word learning

AU - Kalashnikova, Marina

AU - Mattock, Karen Janette

AU - Monaghan, Padraic John

PY - 2016

Y1 - 2016

N2 - From an early age, children apply the mutual exclusivity (ME) assumption, demonstrating preference for one-to-one mappings between words and their referents. However, for the acquisition of referentially overlapping terms, ME use must be suspended. We test whether contextual cues to intended meaning, in the form of presence of a speaker, may be critical for flexible ME application. Four- to five-year-old children were tested on two word learning tasks requiring flexible use of ME, respectively. In Experiment 1, children saw video recordings of the speakers introducing the novel labels. All children successfully applied ME and succeeded in accepting lexical overlap. In Experiment 2, with audio recordings of speakers only, children were unsuccessful at accepting lexical overlap. Thus, flexible use of ME relies on a developing ability to utilise the contextual information present in communicative interactions.

AB - From an early age, children apply the mutual exclusivity (ME) assumption, demonstrating preference for one-to-one mappings between words and their referents. However, for the acquisition of referentially overlapping terms, ME use must be suspended. We test whether contextual cues to intended meaning, in the form of presence of a speaker, may be critical for flexible ME application. Four- to five-year-old children were tested on two word learning tasks requiring flexible use of ME, respectively. In Experiment 1, children saw video recordings of the speakers introducing the novel labels. All children successfully applied ME and succeeded in accepting lexical overlap. In Experiment 2, with audio recordings of speakers only, children were unsuccessful at accepting lexical overlap. Thus, flexible use of ME relies on a developing ability to utilise the contextual information present in communicative interactions.

U2 - 10.1080/15475441.2015.1023443

DO - 10.1080/15475441.2015.1023443

M3 - Journal article

VL - 12

SP - 79

EP - 91

JO - Language Learning and Development

JF - Language Learning and Development

SN - 1547-5441

IS - 1

ER -