Home > Research > Publications & Outputs > Inference neglect and ignorance denial.
View graph of relations

Inference neglect and ignorance denial.

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Published

Standard

Inference neglect and ignorance denial. / Vrouxaki, A.; Freeman, N. H.; Peters, D. et al.
In: British Journal of Developmental Psychology, Vol. 17, No. 4, 11.1999, p. 483-499.

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Harvard

Vrouxaki, A, Freeman, NH, Peters, D & Lewis, C 1999, 'Inference neglect and ignorance denial.', British Journal of Developmental Psychology, vol. 17, no. 4, pp. 483-499. <http://bpsoc.publisher.ingentaconnect.com/content/bpsoc/bjdp/1999/00000017/00000004/art00001>

APA

Vrouxaki, A., Freeman, N. H., Peters, D., & Lewis, C. (1999). Inference neglect and ignorance denial. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17(4), 483-499. http://bpsoc.publisher.ingentaconnect.com/content/bpsoc/bjdp/1999/00000017/00000004/art00001

Vancouver

Vrouxaki A, Freeman NH, Peters D, Lewis C. Inference neglect and ignorance denial. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1999 Nov;17(4):483-499.

Author

Vrouxaki, A. ; Freeman, N. H. ; Peters, D. et al. / Inference neglect and ignorance denial. In: British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1999 ; Vol. 17, No. 4. pp. 483-499.

Bibtex

@article{6271705cd6224d10b289f96883f507bb,
title = "Inference neglect and ignorance denial.",
abstract = "Young children seem to overextend a 'seeing = knowing rule' so that they neglect to notice that people gain knowledge from inferring as well as from seeing. Yet that cannot be a sufficient explanation of children's problems with conceptualizing knowledge, because rule underextension occurs in children's claims to know something that they have not seen (nor inferred). The two errors were studied together with pairs of children aged 4 and 5 years. Each child had their own box, items on the table were shared out into the boxes, and either both children, or neither, or one of them, looked in their own box. Children were asked if they and the other knew what was in each other's box; and were asked for explanations and predictions. About a quarter of the children showed full competence. In others, overextension and underextension occurred; yet almost all children explained that inference was involved in knowing, without bias towards giving such explanation more for self than for other. Error patterns were not predictable from a test for understanding the term 'know'. It is suggested that children have a framework conception of 'knowing' in which another's mind is treated as similar to own mind, but problems arise in implementing that insight before children manage to conceptualize constraints on knowledge.",
author = "A. Vrouxaki and Freeman, {N. H.} and D. Peters and Charlie Lewis",
year = "1999",
month = nov,
language = "English",
volume = "17",
pages = "483--499",
journal = "British Journal of Developmental Psychology",
issn = "0261-510X",
publisher = "Wiley-Blackwell",
number = "4",

}

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - Inference neglect and ignorance denial.

AU - Vrouxaki, A.

AU - Freeman, N. H.

AU - Peters, D.

AU - Lewis, Charlie

PY - 1999/11

Y1 - 1999/11

N2 - Young children seem to overextend a 'seeing = knowing rule' so that they neglect to notice that people gain knowledge from inferring as well as from seeing. Yet that cannot be a sufficient explanation of children's problems with conceptualizing knowledge, because rule underextension occurs in children's claims to know something that they have not seen (nor inferred). The two errors were studied together with pairs of children aged 4 and 5 years. Each child had their own box, items on the table were shared out into the boxes, and either both children, or neither, or one of them, looked in their own box. Children were asked if they and the other knew what was in each other's box; and were asked for explanations and predictions. About a quarter of the children showed full competence. In others, overextension and underextension occurred; yet almost all children explained that inference was involved in knowing, without bias towards giving such explanation more for self than for other. Error patterns were not predictable from a test for understanding the term 'know'. It is suggested that children have a framework conception of 'knowing' in which another's mind is treated as similar to own mind, but problems arise in implementing that insight before children manage to conceptualize constraints on knowledge.

AB - Young children seem to overextend a 'seeing = knowing rule' so that they neglect to notice that people gain knowledge from inferring as well as from seeing. Yet that cannot be a sufficient explanation of children's problems with conceptualizing knowledge, because rule underextension occurs in children's claims to know something that they have not seen (nor inferred). The two errors were studied together with pairs of children aged 4 and 5 years. Each child had their own box, items on the table were shared out into the boxes, and either both children, or neither, or one of them, looked in their own box. Children were asked if they and the other knew what was in each other's box; and were asked for explanations and predictions. About a quarter of the children showed full competence. In others, overextension and underextension occurred; yet almost all children explained that inference was involved in knowing, without bias towards giving such explanation more for self than for other. Error patterns were not predictable from a test for understanding the term 'know'. It is suggested that children have a framework conception of 'knowing' in which another's mind is treated as similar to own mind, but problems arise in implementing that insight before children manage to conceptualize constraints on knowledge.

M3 - Journal article

VL - 17

SP - 483

EP - 499

JO - British Journal of Developmental Psychology

JF - British Journal of Developmental Psychology

SN - 0261-510X

IS - 4

ER -