Home > Research > Publications & Outputs > Multiple mechanisms of word learning in late ta...

Electronic data

Links

Text available via DOI:

View graph of relations

Multiple mechanisms of word learning in late talking children: A longitudinal study

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Published

Standard

Multiple mechanisms of word learning in late talking children: A longitudinal study. / Cheung, Rachael; Hartley, Calum; Monaghan, Padraic.
In: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, Vol. 65, No. 8, 17.08.2022, p. 2978-2995.

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Harvard

Cheung, R, Hartley, C & Monaghan, P 2022, 'Multiple mechanisms of word learning in late talking children: A longitudinal study', Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, vol. 65, no. 8, pp. 2978-2995. https://doi.org/10.1044/2022_JSLHR-21-00610

APA

Vancouver

Cheung R, Hartley C, Monaghan P. Multiple mechanisms of word learning in late talking children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2022 Aug 17;65(8):2978-2995. Epub 2022 Aug 3. doi: 10.1044/2022_JSLHR-21-00610

Author

Cheung, Rachael ; Hartley, Calum ; Monaghan, Padraic. / Multiple mechanisms of word learning in late talking children : A longitudinal study. In: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2022 ; Vol. 65, No. 8. pp. 2978-2995.

Bibtex

@article{d878dfcc715e4efcbba3dfe04ae293f0,
title = "Multiple mechanisms of word learning in late talking children: A longitudinal study",
abstract = "Purpose: To identify variability in word learning mechanisms used by late talking children using a longitudinal study design, which may explain variability in late talking children{\textquoteright}s outcomes. Method: A cohort of typically developing children (n = 40) and children who were classified as late talking children at age 2;0 (10th percentile on expressive vocabulary, n = 21) were followed up at age 3;0 and at age 3;6. We tested the cohort across tasks designed to isolate different mechanisms involved in word learning: encoding and producing spoken forms of words (using a nonword repetition task), identifying referents for words (using a fast mapping task), and learning associations between words and referents (using a cross-situational word learning task).Results: Late talking children had lower accuracy on nonword repetition than typically developing children, despite most of the sample reaching typical ranges for expressive vocabulary at age 3;6. There were no between-group differences in fast mapping and retention accuracy, however, both were predicted by concurrent expressive vocabulary. Late talking children performed less accurately than typically developing children on cross-situational word learning retention trials, despite showing no between-group differences during training trials. Combining performance across all three tasks predicted approximately 45% of the variance in vocabulary outcomes at the last timepoint.Conclusions: Late talking children continue to have deficits in phonological representation that impact their word learning ability and expressive language abilities, but do not show difficulties in fast mapping novel words. Late talking children may also struggle to retain associative information about word-referent mappings. Late talking children thus use some, but not all, word learning mechanisms differently than typically developing children.",
author = "Rachael Cheung and Calum Hartley and Padraic Monaghan",
year = "2022",
month = aug,
day = "17",
doi = "10.1044/2022_JSLHR-21-00610",
language = "English",
volume = "65",
pages = "2978--2995",
journal = "Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research",
issn = "1092-4388",
publisher = "American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA)",
number = "8",

}

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - Multiple mechanisms of word learning in late talking children

T2 - A longitudinal study

AU - Cheung, Rachael

AU - Hartley, Calum

AU - Monaghan, Padraic

PY - 2022/8/17

Y1 - 2022/8/17

N2 - Purpose: To identify variability in word learning mechanisms used by late talking children using a longitudinal study design, which may explain variability in late talking children’s outcomes. Method: A cohort of typically developing children (n = 40) and children who were classified as late talking children at age 2;0 (10th percentile on expressive vocabulary, n = 21) were followed up at age 3;0 and at age 3;6. We tested the cohort across tasks designed to isolate different mechanisms involved in word learning: encoding and producing spoken forms of words (using a nonword repetition task), identifying referents for words (using a fast mapping task), and learning associations between words and referents (using a cross-situational word learning task).Results: Late talking children had lower accuracy on nonword repetition than typically developing children, despite most of the sample reaching typical ranges for expressive vocabulary at age 3;6. There were no between-group differences in fast mapping and retention accuracy, however, both were predicted by concurrent expressive vocabulary. Late talking children performed less accurately than typically developing children on cross-situational word learning retention trials, despite showing no between-group differences during training trials. Combining performance across all three tasks predicted approximately 45% of the variance in vocabulary outcomes at the last timepoint.Conclusions: Late talking children continue to have deficits in phonological representation that impact their word learning ability and expressive language abilities, but do not show difficulties in fast mapping novel words. Late talking children may also struggle to retain associative information about word-referent mappings. Late talking children thus use some, but not all, word learning mechanisms differently than typically developing children.

AB - Purpose: To identify variability in word learning mechanisms used by late talking children using a longitudinal study design, which may explain variability in late talking children’s outcomes. Method: A cohort of typically developing children (n = 40) and children who were classified as late talking children at age 2;0 (10th percentile on expressive vocabulary, n = 21) were followed up at age 3;0 and at age 3;6. We tested the cohort across tasks designed to isolate different mechanisms involved in word learning: encoding and producing spoken forms of words (using a nonword repetition task), identifying referents for words (using a fast mapping task), and learning associations between words and referents (using a cross-situational word learning task).Results: Late talking children had lower accuracy on nonword repetition than typically developing children, despite most of the sample reaching typical ranges for expressive vocabulary at age 3;6. There were no between-group differences in fast mapping and retention accuracy, however, both were predicted by concurrent expressive vocabulary. Late talking children performed less accurately than typically developing children on cross-situational word learning retention trials, despite showing no between-group differences during training trials. Combining performance across all three tasks predicted approximately 45% of the variance in vocabulary outcomes at the last timepoint.Conclusions: Late talking children continue to have deficits in phonological representation that impact their word learning ability and expressive language abilities, but do not show difficulties in fast mapping novel words. Late talking children may also struggle to retain associative information about word-referent mappings. Late talking children thus use some, but not all, word learning mechanisms differently than typically developing children.

U2 - 10.1044/2022_JSLHR-21-00610

DO - 10.1044/2022_JSLHR-21-00610

M3 - Journal article

VL - 65

SP - 2978

EP - 2995

JO - Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research

JF - Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research

SN - 1092-4388

IS - 8

ER -