Home > Research > Publications & Outputs > Pre-emptive interaction in language change and ...

Electronic data

Text available via DOI:

View graph of relations

Pre-emptive interaction in language change and ontogeny: the case of [there is no NP]

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Published

Standard

Pre-emptive interaction in language change and ontogeny: the case of [there is no NP]. / Tantucci, Vittorio; Di Cristofaro, Matteo.
In: Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, Vol. 17, No. 3, 30.11.2021, p. 715-742.

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Harvard

Tantucci, V & Di Cristofaro, M 2021, 'Pre-emptive interaction in language change and ontogeny: the case of [there is no NP]', Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, vol. 17, no. 3, pp. 715-742. https://doi.org/10.1515/cllt-2020-0007

APA

Tantucci, V., & Di Cristofaro, M. (2021). Pre-emptive interaction in language change and ontogeny: the case of [there is no NP]. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 17(3), 715-742. https://doi.org/10.1515/cllt-2020-0007

Vancouver

Tantucci V, Di Cristofaro M. Pre-emptive interaction in language change and ontogeny: the case of [there is no NP]. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory. 2021 Nov 30;17(3):715-742. Epub 2020 Dec 22. doi: 10.1515/cllt-2020-0007

Author

Tantucci, Vittorio ; Di Cristofaro, Matteo. / Pre-emptive interaction in language change and ontogeny : the case of [there is no NP]. In: Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory. 2021 ; Vol. 17, No. 3. pp. 715-742.

Bibtex

@article{38e2913702e04a93986202fb26ea526f,
title = "Pre-emptive interaction in language change and ontogeny: the case of [there is no NP]",
abstract = "This study is centred on the pre-emptive dimension of interactional exchanges. Dialogues are not merely characterised by information transmission, they are also constantly informed by pre-emptive attempts to address potential reactions to what is being said. We argue that pre-emptive interaction intersects with intersubjectivity (i.a. Traugott, Elizabeth C. 2003. From subjectification to intersubjectification. In R. Hickey (ed.), Motives for language change, 124–139. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; Schwenter, Scott A. & Richard Waltereit. 2010. Presupposition accommodation and language change. In K. Davidse & L. Vandelanotte (eds.), Subjectification, intersubjectification and grammaticalization, 75–102. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton; Tantucci, Vittorio. 2017a. From immediate to extended intersubjectification: A gradient approach to intersubjective awareness and semasiological change. Language and Cognition 9(1). 88–120; Tantucci, Vittorio. 2020. From co-actionality to extended intersubjectivity: Drawing on language change and ontogenetic development. Applied Linguistics 41(2). 185–214) and constitutes an important trigger of semantic-pragmatic reanalysis and constructional change. We provide a corpus-based study centred on the change of the [there is no NP] construction in Early Modern English dialogic interaction. During 16th century, the chunk is originally used in assertions, however it then progressively acquires a new function of pre-emptive refusal. Something similar is at stake throughout the child{\textquoteright}s ontogeny. We provide corpus-based data from the CHILDES database of first language acquisition to show that children{\textquoteright}s ability to use [there is no NP] to address potential reactions to what is being said occurs only around the fourth year of age, that is when a Theory of Mind (ToM) starts to become fully developed (i.a. Apperly, Ian. 2010. Mindreaders: The cognitive basis of theory of mind. New York: Psychology Press; Wellman, Henry M. 2014. Making minds: How theory of mind develops. Oxford: Oxford University Press). Pre-emptive interaction correlates diachronically and ontogentically with ToM and underpins a projected turn taking of a specific or generic interlocutor as a result of what is being currently said.",
keywords = "assertion, corpus based, reanalysis, speech act, theory of mind, intersubjectivity, first language acquisition, ontogeny, language change",
author = "Vittorio Tantucci and {Di Cristofaro}, Matteo",
year = "2021",
month = nov,
day = "30",
doi = "10.1515/cllt-2020-0007",
language = "English",
volume = "17",
pages = "715--742",
journal = "Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory",
issn = "1613-7027",
publisher = "Walter de Gruyter GmbH & Co. KG",
number = "3",

}

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - Pre-emptive interaction in language change and ontogeny

T2 - the case of [there is no NP]

AU - Tantucci, Vittorio

AU - Di Cristofaro, Matteo

PY - 2021/11/30

Y1 - 2021/11/30

N2 - This study is centred on the pre-emptive dimension of interactional exchanges. Dialogues are not merely characterised by information transmission, they are also constantly informed by pre-emptive attempts to address potential reactions to what is being said. We argue that pre-emptive interaction intersects with intersubjectivity (i.a. Traugott, Elizabeth C. 2003. From subjectification to intersubjectification. In R. Hickey (ed.), Motives for language change, 124–139. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; Schwenter, Scott A. & Richard Waltereit. 2010. Presupposition accommodation and language change. In K. Davidse & L. Vandelanotte (eds.), Subjectification, intersubjectification and grammaticalization, 75–102. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton; Tantucci, Vittorio. 2017a. From immediate to extended intersubjectification: A gradient approach to intersubjective awareness and semasiological change. Language and Cognition 9(1). 88–120; Tantucci, Vittorio. 2020. From co-actionality to extended intersubjectivity: Drawing on language change and ontogenetic development. Applied Linguistics 41(2). 185–214) and constitutes an important trigger of semantic-pragmatic reanalysis and constructional change. We provide a corpus-based study centred on the change of the [there is no NP] construction in Early Modern English dialogic interaction. During 16th century, the chunk is originally used in assertions, however it then progressively acquires a new function of pre-emptive refusal. Something similar is at stake throughout the child’s ontogeny. We provide corpus-based data from the CHILDES database of first language acquisition to show that children’s ability to use [there is no NP] to address potential reactions to what is being said occurs only around the fourth year of age, that is when a Theory of Mind (ToM) starts to become fully developed (i.a. Apperly, Ian. 2010. Mindreaders: The cognitive basis of theory of mind. New York: Psychology Press; Wellman, Henry M. 2014. Making minds: How theory of mind develops. Oxford: Oxford University Press). Pre-emptive interaction correlates diachronically and ontogentically with ToM and underpins a projected turn taking of a specific or generic interlocutor as a result of what is being currently said.

AB - This study is centred on the pre-emptive dimension of interactional exchanges. Dialogues are not merely characterised by information transmission, they are also constantly informed by pre-emptive attempts to address potential reactions to what is being said. We argue that pre-emptive interaction intersects with intersubjectivity (i.a. Traugott, Elizabeth C. 2003. From subjectification to intersubjectification. In R. Hickey (ed.), Motives for language change, 124–139. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; Schwenter, Scott A. & Richard Waltereit. 2010. Presupposition accommodation and language change. In K. Davidse & L. Vandelanotte (eds.), Subjectification, intersubjectification and grammaticalization, 75–102. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton; Tantucci, Vittorio. 2017a. From immediate to extended intersubjectification: A gradient approach to intersubjective awareness and semasiological change. Language and Cognition 9(1). 88–120; Tantucci, Vittorio. 2020. From co-actionality to extended intersubjectivity: Drawing on language change and ontogenetic development. Applied Linguistics 41(2). 185–214) and constitutes an important trigger of semantic-pragmatic reanalysis and constructional change. We provide a corpus-based study centred on the change of the [there is no NP] construction in Early Modern English dialogic interaction. During 16th century, the chunk is originally used in assertions, however it then progressively acquires a new function of pre-emptive refusal. Something similar is at stake throughout the child’s ontogeny. We provide corpus-based data from the CHILDES database of first language acquisition to show that children’s ability to use [there is no NP] to address potential reactions to what is being said occurs only around the fourth year of age, that is when a Theory of Mind (ToM) starts to become fully developed (i.a. Apperly, Ian. 2010. Mindreaders: The cognitive basis of theory of mind. New York: Psychology Press; Wellman, Henry M. 2014. Making minds: How theory of mind develops. Oxford: Oxford University Press). Pre-emptive interaction correlates diachronically and ontogentically with ToM and underpins a projected turn taking of a specific or generic interlocutor as a result of what is being currently said.

KW - assertion

KW - corpus based

KW - reanalysis

KW - speech act

KW - theory of mind

KW - intersubjectivity

KW - first language acquisition

KW - ontogeny

KW - language change

U2 - 10.1515/cllt-2020-0007

DO - 10.1515/cllt-2020-0007

M3 - Journal article

VL - 17

SP - 715

EP - 742

JO - Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory

JF - Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory

SN - 1613-7027

IS - 3

ER -