Home > Research > Publications & Outputs > Rationalizing meat consumption

Associated organisational unit

Links

Text available via DOI:

View graph of relations

Rationalizing meat consumption: the 4Ns

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Published

Standard

Rationalizing meat consumption: the 4Ns. / Piazza, Jared; Ruby, Matt; Loughnan, Stephen et al.
In: Appetite, Vol. 91, 01.08.2015, p. 114-128.

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Harvard

Piazza, J, Ruby, M, Loughnan, S, Luong, M, Kulik, J, Watkins, H & Seigerman, M 2015, 'Rationalizing meat consumption: the 4Ns', Appetite, vol. 91, pp. 114-128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.04.011

APA

Piazza, J., Ruby, M., Loughnan, S., Luong, M., Kulik, J., Watkins, H., & Seigerman, M. (2015). Rationalizing meat consumption: the 4Ns. Appetite, 91, 114-128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.04.011

Vancouver

Piazza J, Ruby M, Loughnan S, Luong M, Kulik J, Watkins H et al. Rationalizing meat consumption: the 4Ns. Appetite. 2015 Aug 1;91:114-128. Epub 2015 Apr 9. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.04.011

Author

Piazza, Jared ; Ruby, Matt ; Loughnan, Stephen et al. / Rationalizing meat consumption : the 4Ns. In: Appetite. 2015 ; Vol. 91. pp. 114-128.

Bibtex

@article{0c447df6608a42d2ad24cc5b211c9bb8,
title = "Rationalizing meat consumption: the 4Ns",
abstract = "Recent theorizing suggests the 4Ns—that is, the belief that eating meat is natural, normal, necessary, and nice—are common rationalizations people use to defend their choice of eating meat. However, such theorizing has yet to be subjected to empirical testing. Six studies were conducted on the 4Ns. Studies 1a-1b demonstrated that the 4N classification captures the vast majority (83%-91%) of justifications people naturally offer in defense of eating meat. In Study 2, individuals who endorsed the 4Ns tended also to objectify (dementalize) animals and included fewer animals in their circle of moral concern, and this was true independent of social dominance orientation. Subsequent studies (Studies 3-5) showed that individuals who endorsed the 4Ns tend not to be motivated by ethical concerns when making food choices, are less involved in animal-welfare advocacy, less driven to restrict animal products from their diet, less proud of their animal-product decisions, tend to endorse Speciesist attitudes, tend to consume meat and animal products more frequently, and are highly committed to eating meat. Furthermore, omnivores who strongly endorsed the 4Ns tended to experience less guilt about their animal-product decisions, highlighting the guilt-alleviating function of the 4Ns.",
keywords = "Meat, Vegetarianism, Rationalization, Justification, Animal welfare, Attitudes",
author = "Jared Piazza and Matt Ruby and Stephen Loughnan and Mischel Luong and Juliana Kulik and Hanne Watkins and Mirra Seigerman",
year = "2015",
month = aug,
day = "1",
doi = "10.1016/j.appet.2015.04.011",
language = "English",
volume = "91",
pages = "114--128",
journal = "Appetite",
issn = "0195-6663",
publisher = "ACADEMIC PRESS LTD- ELSEVIER SCIENCE LTD",

}

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - Rationalizing meat consumption

T2 - the 4Ns

AU - Piazza, Jared

AU - Ruby, Matt

AU - Loughnan, Stephen

AU - Luong, Mischel

AU - Kulik, Juliana

AU - Watkins, Hanne

AU - Seigerman, Mirra

PY - 2015/8/1

Y1 - 2015/8/1

N2 - Recent theorizing suggests the 4Ns—that is, the belief that eating meat is natural, normal, necessary, and nice—are common rationalizations people use to defend their choice of eating meat. However, such theorizing has yet to be subjected to empirical testing. Six studies were conducted on the 4Ns. Studies 1a-1b demonstrated that the 4N classification captures the vast majority (83%-91%) of justifications people naturally offer in defense of eating meat. In Study 2, individuals who endorsed the 4Ns tended also to objectify (dementalize) animals and included fewer animals in their circle of moral concern, and this was true independent of social dominance orientation. Subsequent studies (Studies 3-5) showed that individuals who endorsed the 4Ns tend not to be motivated by ethical concerns when making food choices, are less involved in animal-welfare advocacy, less driven to restrict animal products from their diet, less proud of their animal-product decisions, tend to endorse Speciesist attitudes, tend to consume meat and animal products more frequently, and are highly committed to eating meat. Furthermore, omnivores who strongly endorsed the 4Ns tended to experience less guilt about their animal-product decisions, highlighting the guilt-alleviating function of the 4Ns.

AB - Recent theorizing suggests the 4Ns—that is, the belief that eating meat is natural, normal, necessary, and nice—are common rationalizations people use to defend their choice of eating meat. However, such theorizing has yet to be subjected to empirical testing. Six studies were conducted on the 4Ns. Studies 1a-1b demonstrated that the 4N classification captures the vast majority (83%-91%) of justifications people naturally offer in defense of eating meat. In Study 2, individuals who endorsed the 4Ns tended also to objectify (dementalize) animals and included fewer animals in their circle of moral concern, and this was true independent of social dominance orientation. Subsequent studies (Studies 3-5) showed that individuals who endorsed the 4Ns tend not to be motivated by ethical concerns when making food choices, are less involved in animal-welfare advocacy, less driven to restrict animal products from their diet, less proud of their animal-product decisions, tend to endorse Speciesist attitudes, tend to consume meat and animal products more frequently, and are highly committed to eating meat. Furthermore, omnivores who strongly endorsed the 4Ns tended to experience less guilt about their animal-product decisions, highlighting the guilt-alleviating function of the 4Ns.

KW - Meat

KW - Vegetarianism

KW - Rationalization

KW - Justification

KW - Animal welfare

KW - Attitudes

U2 - 10.1016/j.appet.2015.04.011

DO - 10.1016/j.appet.2015.04.011

M3 - Journal article

VL - 91

SP - 114

EP - 128

JO - Appetite

JF - Appetite

SN - 0195-6663

ER -