Home > Research > Publications & Outputs > Sequential effects and sequence learning in a t...

Associated organisational unit

Links

Text available via DOI:

View graph of relations

Sequential effects and sequence learning in a three-choice serial reaction time task

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Published

Standard

Sequential effects and sequence learning in a three-choice serial reaction time task. / Lee, J.C.; Beesley, T.; Livesey, E.J.
In: Acta Psychologica, Vol. 170, 10.2016, p. 168-176.

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Harvard

APA

Vancouver

Lee JC, Beesley T, Livesey EJ. Sequential effects and sequence learning in a three-choice serial reaction time task. Acta Psychologica. 2016 Oct;170:168-176. Epub 2016 Aug 9. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.08.004

Author

Lee, J.C. ; Beesley, T. ; Livesey, E.J. / Sequential effects and sequence learning in a three-choice serial reaction time task. In: Acta Psychologica. 2016 ; Vol. 170. pp. 168-176.

Bibtex

@article{208b242bc9ca43c39a55e3d4b1d95a68,
title = "Sequential effects and sequence learning in a three-choice serial reaction time task",
abstract = "The recent history of events can influence responding despite there being no contingent relationship between those events. These {\textquoteleft}sequential effects{\textquoteright} are ubiquitous in cognitive psychology, yet their study has been dominated by two-choice reaction time tasks in which sequences necessarily comprise simple response repetitions and alternations. The current study explored sequential effects in a three-choice reaction time task where the target was constrained to either move clockwise or anticlockwise on each trial, allowing for assessment of sequential effects involving the direction of target transitions rather than target location. Across two experiments, a reliable pattern of sequential effects was found in the absence of contingencies, whereby the most notable feature was that participants were fastest to respond to subsequences where the target moved in a consistent direction on consecutive trials, compared to when the target direction alternated. In Experiment 2, the direction of motion was biased to move in one direction 75% of the time and in a subsequent transfer phase, participants showed evidence of learning this probabilistic sequence but still exhibited the same pattern of sequential effects on trials where the target moved in the more prevalent or less prevalent direction. Simulations with a connectionist model of sequence learning (the Augmented Serial Recurrent Network, Cleeremans & McClelland, 1991) produced an adequate replication of the sequential effects in both experiments in addition to an effect of sequence learning in Experiment 2. We propose that sequential effects may represent learning about transient contingencies and may be described using the same associative learning mechanisms intended for sequence learning.",
keywords = "Sequential effect, Sequence learning, Serial reaction time, Serial recurrent network, Contingency",
author = "J.C. Lee and T. Beesley and E.J. Livesey",
note = "cited By 0",
year = "2016",
month = oct,
doi = "10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.08.004",
language = "English",
volume = "170",
pages = "168--176",
journal = "Acta Psychologica",
issn = "0001-6918",
publisher = "Elsevier",

}

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - Sequential effects and sequence learning in a three-choice serial reaction time task

AU - Lee, J.C.

AU - Beesley, T.

AU - Livesey, E.J.

N1 - cited By 0

PY - 2016/10

Y1 - 2016/10

N2 - The recent history of events can influence responding despite there being no contingent relationship between those events. These ‘sequential effects’ are ubiquitous in cognitive psychology, yet their study has been dominated by two-choice reaction time tasks in which sequences necessarily comprise simple response repetitions and alternations. The current study explored sequential effects in a three-choice reaction time task where the target was constrained to either move clockwise or anticlockwise on each trial, allowing for assessment of sequential effects involving the direction of target transitions rather than target location. Across two experiments, a reliable pattern of sequential effects was found in the absence of contingencies, whereby the most notable feature was that participants were fastest to respond to subsequences where the target moved in a consistent direction on consecutive trials, compared to when the target direction alternated. In Experiment 2, the direction of motion was biased to move in one direction 75% of the time and in a subsequent transfer phase, participants showed evidence of learning this probabilistic sequence but still exhibited the same pattern of sequential effects on trials where the target moved in the more prevalent or less prevalent direction. Simulations with a connectionist model of sequence learning (the Augmented Serial Recurrent Network, Cleeremans & McClelland, 1991) produced an adequate replication of the sequential effects in both experiments in addition to an effect of sequence learning in Experiment 2. We propose that sequential effects may represent learning about transient contingencies and may be described using the same associative learning mechanisms intended for sequence learning.

AB - The recent history of events can influence responding despite there being no contingent relationship between those events. These ‘sequential effects’ are ubiquitous in cognitive psychology, yet their study has been dominated by two-choice reaction time tasks in which sequences necessarily comprise simple response repetitions and alternations. The current study explored sequential effects in a three-choice reaction time task where the target was constrained to either move clockwise or anticlockwise on each trial, allowing for assessment of sequential effects involving the direction of target transitions rather than target location. Across two experiments, a reliable pattern of sequential effects was found in the absence of contingencies, whereby the most notable feature was that participants were fastest to respond to subsequences where the target moved in a consistent direction on consecutive trials, compared to when the target direction alternated. In Experiment 2, the direction of motion was biased to move in one direction 75% of the time and in a subsequent transfer phase, participants showed evidence of learning this probabilistic sequence but still exhibited the same pattern of sequential effects on trials where the target moved in the more prevalent or less prevalent direction. Simulations with a connectionist model of sequence learning (the Augmented Serial Recurrent Network, Cleeremans & McClelland, 1991) produced an adequate replication of the sequential effects in both experiments in addition to an effect of sequence learning in Experiment 2. We propose that sequential effects may represent learning about transient contingencies and may be described using the same associative learning mechanisms intended for sequence learning.

KW - Sequential effect

KW - Sequence learning

KW - Serial reaction time

KW - Serial recurrent network

KW - Contingency

U2 - 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.08.004

DO - 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.08.004

M3 - Journal article

VL - 170

SP - 168

EP - 176

JO - Acta Psychologica

JF - Acta Psychologica

SN - 0001-6918

ER -