Home > Research > Publications & Outputs > The role of morphological markedness in the pro...

Links

Text available via DOI:

View graph of relations

The role of morphological markedness in the processing of number and gender agreement in Spanish: an event-related potential investigation

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Published

Standard

The role of morphological markedness in the processing of number and gender agreement in Spanish: an event-related potential investigation. / Alemán Bañón, José; Rothman, Jason.
In: Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, Vol. 31, No. 10, 25.11.2016, p. 1273-1298.

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Harvard

APA

Vancouver

Alemán Bañón J, Rothman J. The role of morphological markedness in the processing of number and gender agreement in Spanish: an event-related potential investigation. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience. 2016 Nov 25;31(10):1273-1298. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1218032

Author

Alemán Bañón, José ; Rothman, Jason. / The role of morphological markedness in the processing of number and gender agreement in Spanish : an event-related potential investigation. In: Language, Cognition and Neuroscience. 2016 ; Vol. 31, No. 10. pp. 1273-1298.

Bibtex

@article{98a394af9247496e97211a70e0bc9c73,
title = "The role of morphological markedness in the processing of number and gender agreement in Spanish: an event-related potential investigation",
abstract = "Current morphological theory assumes that feature values, such as masculine and feminine or singular and plural, are asymmetrically represented. That is, one member of the opposition (e.g. feminine for gender, plural for number) is assumed to be marked, and the other one, unmarked. The present study examines how these asymmetries impact agreement resolution in Spanish. Agreement was manipulated between a noun acting as head of a relative clause and an adjective located inside the relative clause (e.g. catedral que parec{\'i}a inmensa “cathedral that looked huge”). Half of the nouns were feminine (marked) and the other half, masculine (unmarked). Half of the nouns were used in the plural (marked) and the other half, in the singular (unmarked). Twenty-seven Spanish native speakers read 240 sentences while their brain activity was recorded with electroencephalography and performed a grammaticality judgment. Results showed that both number and gender violations elicited a central-posterior P600, a component associated with syntactic repair, and a late anterior negativity, argued to reflect working memory costs. Only the P600 was affected by markedness. It started earlier for violations where the mismatching feature was marked. Moreover, it was larger for errors where the mismatching feature was marked, although this amplitude modulation only emerged for number, possibly due to differences in how number and gender cues were realized (i.e. both masculine and feminine showed overt inflection, but singular was uninflected relative to plural). These results suggest that the parser is sensitive to markedness asymmetries in the course of online processing.",
keywords = "agreement, markedness, number and gender, P600",
author = "{Alem{\'a}n Ba{\~n}{\'o}n}, Jos{\'e} and Jason Rothman",
year = "2016",
month = nov,
day = "25",
doi = "10.1080/23273798.2016.1218032",
language = "English",
volume = "31",
pages = "1273--1298",
journal = "Language, Cognition and Neuroscience",
issn = "2327-3798",
publisher = "Taylor and Francis",
number = "10",

}

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - The role of morphological markedness in the processing of number and gender agreement in Spanish

T2 - an event-related potential investigation

AU - Alemán Bañón, José

AU - Rothman, Jason

PY - 2016/11/25

Y1 - 2016/11/25

N2 - Current morphological theory assumes that feature values, such as masculine and feminine or singular and plural, are asymmetrically represented. That is, one member of the opposition (e.g. feminine for gender, plural for number) is assumed to be marked, and the other one, unmarked. The present study examines how these asymmetries impact agreement resolution in Spanish. Agreement was manipulated between a noun acting as head of a relative clause and an adjective located inside the relative clause (e.g. catedral que parecía inmensa “cathedral that looked huge”). Half of the nouns were feminine (marked) and the other half, masculine (unmarked). Half of the nouns were used in the plural (marked) and the other half, in the singular (unmarked). Twenty-seven Spanish native speakers read 240 sentences while their brain activity was recorded with electroencephalography and performed a grammaticality judgment. Results showed that both number and gender violations elicited a central-posterior P600, a component associated with syntactic repair, and a late anterior negativity, argued to reflect working memory costs. Only the P600 was affected by markedness. It started earlier for violations where the mismatching feature was marked. Moreover, it was larger for errors where the mismatching feature was marked, although this amplitude modulation only emerged for number, possibly due to differences in how number and gender cues were realized (i.e. both masculine and feminine showed overt inflection, but singular was uninflected relative to plural). These results suggest that the parser is sensitive to markedness asymmetries in the course of online processing.

AB - Current morphological theory assumes that feature values, such as masculine and feminine or singular and plural, are asymmetrically represented. That is, one member of the opposition (e.g. feminine for gender, plural for number) is assumed to be marked, and the other one, unmarked. The present study examines how these asymmetries impact agreement resolution in Spanish. Agreement was manipulated between a noun acting as head of a relative clause and an adjective located inside the relative clause (e.g. catedral que parecía inmensa “cathedral that looked huge”). Half of the nouns were feminine (marked) and the other half, masculine (unmarked). Half of the nouns were used in the plural (marked) and the other half, in the singular (unmarked). Twenty-seven Spanish native speakers read 240 sentences while their brain activity was recorded with electroencephalography and performed a grammaticality judgment. Results showed that both number and gender violations elicited a central-posterior P600, a component associated with syntactic repair, and a late anterior negativity, argued to reflect working memory costs. Only the P600 was affected by markedness. It started earlier for violations where the mismatching feature was marked. Moreover, it was larger for errors where the mismatching feature was marked, although this amplitude modulation only emerged for number, possibly due to differences in how number and gender cues were realized (i.e. both masculine and feminine showed overt inflection, but singular was uninflected relative to plural). These results suggest that the parser is sensitive to markedness asymmetries in the course of online processing.

KW - agreement

KW - markedness

KW - number and gender

KW - P600

U2 - 10.1080/23273798.2016.1218032

DO - 10.1080/23273798.2016.1218032

M3 - Journal article

AN - SCOPUS:84981725614

VL - 31

SP - 1273

EP - 1298

JO - Language, Cognition and Neuroscience

JF - Language, Cognition and Neuroscience

SN - 2327-3798

IS - 10

ER -