Home > Research > Publications & Outputs > When is an object not an object?

Links

Text available via DOI:

View graph of relations

When is an object not an object?: Insights from infants

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineMeeting abstractpeer-review

Published

Standard

When is an object not an object? Insights from infants. / Johnson, S. P.; Bremner, J. G.; Slater, A. et al.
In: Journal of Vision, Vol. 1, No. 3, 459, 01.12.2001.

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineMeeting abstractpeer-review

Harvard

APA

Vancouver

Johnson SP, Bremner JG, Slater A, Mason U, Foster K. When is an object not an object? Insights from infants. Journal of Vision. 2001 Dec 1;1(3):459. doi: 10.1167/1.3.459

Author

Johnson, S. P. ; Bremner, J. G. ; Slater, A. et al. / When is an object not an object? Insights from infants. In: Journal of Vision. 2001 ; Vol. 1, No. 3.

Bibtex

@article{fa2b636c1644490083f30ac521170f28,
title = "When is an object not an object?: Insights from infants",
abstract = "The visual system can track several objects on multiple trajectories, even when temporarily occluded. The ontogenetic origins of these processes have been proposed to lie in perception of objects as spatiotemporally bounded and continuous ({"}object continuity{"}) from the start in infancy. Using habituation and eye tracking methods, however, we have obtained data indicating that young infants are poor at perceiving objects across occlusion. After habituation to a ball that moved repeatedly behind an occluding box and out again (occlusion time 733 ms), 4-month-olds looked longer at a {"}complete{"} trajectory, in which the ball moved back and forth with no occluder, relative to a {"}broken{"} trajectory, in which it disappeared and then reappeared. This preference reversed when the ball was occluded for a shorter time (67 ms). Six-month-olds, however, responded to object continuity for the longer occlusion duration, and 2-month-olds performed poorly even at the shortest duration, suggesting that object continuity emerges gradually between birth and 6 months. Using an eye tracker, we found that 4-month-olds rarely anticipate the ball's re-emergence, although performance in this condition was better than chance, suggesting some level of understanding object continuity. Six-month-olds anticipate on significantly more trials. When 4-month-olds first viewed the moving ball with no occluder, however, anticipations were much more reliable, suggesting that one mechanism of development of object continuity is simply experience seeing objects move. Object continuity, therefore, does not seem to be an innate visual function. Instead, experience viewing objects in motion may be crucial for its development.",
author = "Johnson, {S. P.} and Bremner, {J. G.} and A. Slater and U. Mason and K. Foster",
year = "2001",
month = dec,
day = "1",
doi = "10.1167/1.3.459",
language = "English",
volume = "1",
journal = "Journal of Vision",
issn = "1534-7362",
publisher = "Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Inc.",
number = "3",

}

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - When is an object not an object?

T2 - Insights from infants

AU - Johnson, S. P.

AU - Bremner, J. G.

AU - Slater, A.

AU - Mason, U.

AU - Foster, K.

PY - 2001/12/1

Y1 - 2001/12/1

N2 - The visual system can track several objects on multiple trajectories, even when temporarily occluded. The ontogenetic origins of these processes have been proposed to lie in perception of objects as spatiotemporally bounded and continuous ("object continuity") from the start in infancy. Using habituation and eye tracking methods, however, we have obtained data indicating that young infants are poor at perceiving objects across occlusion. After habituation to a ball that moved repeatedly behind an occluding box and out again (occlusion time 733 ms), 4-month-olds looked longer at a "complete" trajectory, in which the ball moved back and forth with no occluder, relative to a "broken" trajectory, in which it disappeared and then reappeared. This preference reversed when the ball was occluded for a shorter time (67 ms). Six-month-olds, however, responded to object continuity for the longer occlusion duration, and 2-month-olds performed poorly even at the shortest duration, suggesting that object continuity emerges gradually between birth and 6 months. Using an eye tracker, we found that 4-month-olds rarely anticipate the ball's re-emergence, although performance in this condition was better than chance, suggesting some level of understanding object continuity. Six-month-olds anticipate on significantly more trials. When 4-month-olds first viewed the moving ball with no occluder, however, anticipations were much more reliable, suggesting that one mechanism of development of object continuity is simply experience seeing objects move. Object continuity, therefore, does not seem to be an innate visual function. Instead, experience viewing objects in motion may be crucial for its development.

AB - The visual system can track several objects on multiple trajectories, even when temporarily occluded. The ontogenetic origins of these processes have been proposed to lie in perception of objects as spatiotemporally bounded and continuous ("object continuity") from the start in infancy. Using habituation and eye tracking methods, however, we have obtained data indicating that young infants are poor at perceiving objects across occlusion. After habituation to a ball that moved repeatedly behind an occluding box and out again (occlusion time 733 ms), 4-month-olds looked longer at a "complete" trajectory, in which the ball moved back and forth with no occluder, relative to a "broken" trajectory, in which it disappeared and then reappeared. This preference reversed when the ball was occluded for a shorter time (67 ms). Six-month-olds, however, responded to object continuity for the longer occlusion duration, and 2-month-olds performed poorly even at the shortest duration, suggesting that object continuity emerges gradually between birth and 6 months. Using an eye tracker, we found that 4-month-olds rarely anticipate the ball's re-emergence, although performance in this condition was better than chance, suggesting some level of understanding object continuity. Six-month-olds anticipate on significantly more trials. When 4-month-olds first viewed the moving ball with no occluder, however, anticipations were much more reliable, suggesting that one mechanism of development of object continuity is simply experience seeing objects move. Object continuity, therefore, does not seem to be an innate visual function. Instead, experience viewing objects in motion may be crucial for its development.

U2 - 10.1167/1.3.459

DO - 10.1167/1.3.459

M3 - Meeting abstract

AN - SCOPUS:4143137633

VL - 1

JO - Journal of Vision

JF - Journal of Vision

SN - 1534-7362

IS - 3

M1 - 459

ER -