Home > Research > Publications & Outputs > You cannot hide your telephone lies

Links

Text available via DOI:

View graph of relations

You cannot hide your telephone lies: providing a model statement as an aid to detect deception in insurance telephone calls

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Published

Standard

You cannot hide your telephone lies: providing a model statement as an aid to detect deception in insurance telephone calls. / Leal, Sharon ; Vrij, Aldert; Warmelink, Lara et al.
In: Legal and Criminological Psychology, Vol. 20, No. 1, 02.2015, p. 129-146.

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Harvard

Leal, S, Vrij, A, Warmelink, L, Vernham, Z & Fisher, RP 2015, 'You cannot hide your telephone lies: providing a model statement as an aid to detect deception in insurance telephone calls', Legal and Criminological Psychology, vol. 20, no. 1, pp. 129-146. https://doi.org/10.1111/lcrp.12017

APA

Vancouver

Leal S, Vrij A, Warmelink L, Vernham Z, Fisher RP. You cannot hide your telephone lies: providing a model statement as an aid to detect deception in insurance telephone calls. Legal and Criminological Psychology. 2015 Feb;20(1):129-146. Epub 2013 May 16. doi: 10.1111/lcrp.12017

Author

Leal, Sharon ; Vrij, Aldert ; Warmelink, Lara et al. / You cannot hide your telephone lies : providing a model statement as an aid to detect deception in insurance telephone calls. In: Legal and Criminological Psychology. 2015 ; Vol. 20, No. 1. pp. 129-146.

Bibtex

@article{898186573d1b4ddca7ef12a7816769e2,
title = "You cannot hide your telephone lies: providing a model statement as an aid to detect deception in insurance telephone calls",
abstract = "Deception research regarding insurance claims is rare but relevant given the financial loss in terms of fraud. In Study 1, a field study in a large multinational insurance fraud detection company, truth telling mock claimants (N = 19) and lying mock claimants (N = 21) were interviewed by insurance company telephone operators. These operators classified correctly only 50% of these truthful and lying claimants, but their task was particularly challenging: Claimants said little, and truthful and deceptive statements did not differ in quality (measured with Criteria-Based Content Analysis [CBCA]) or plausibility. In Study 2, a laboratory experiment, participants in the experimental condition (N = 43) were exposed to an audiotaped truthful and detailed account of an event that was unrelated toinsurance claims (a day at the motor races). The number of words, quality of thestatement (measured with CBCA), and plausibility of the participants{\textquoteright} accounts were compared with participants who were not given a model statement (N = 40). The participants who had listened to the model statement provided longer statements than control participants, truth tellers obtained higher CBCA scores than liars, and only in the model statement condition did truth tellers sound more plausible than liars. Providing participants with a model statement is thus an innovative and successful tool to elicit cues to deception. Providing such a model has the potential to enhance performance in insurance call interviews, and, as we argue, in many other interview settings.",
author = "Sharon Leal and Aldert Vrij and Lara Warmelink and Zarah Vernham and Fisher, {Ronald P.}",
year = "2015",
month = feb,
doi = "10.1111/lcrp.12017",
language = "English",
volume = "20",
pages = "129--146",
journal = "Legal and Criminological Psychology",
issn = "1355-3259",
publisher = "Wiley-Blackwell",
number = "1",

}

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - You cannot hide your telephone lies

T2 - providing a model statement as an aid to detect deception in insurance telephone calls

AU - Leal, Sharon

AU - Vrij, Aldert

AU - Warmelink, Lara

AU - Vernham, Zarah

AU - Fisher, Ronald P.

PY - 2015/2

Y1 - 2015/2

N2 - Deception research regarding insurance claims is rare but relevant given the financial loss in terms of fraud. In Study 1, a field study in a large multinational insurance fraud detection company, truth telling mock claimants (N = 19) and lying mock claimants (N = 21) were interviewed by insurance company telephone operators. These operators classified correctly only 50% of these truthful and lying claimants, but their task was particularly challenging: Claimants said little, and truthful and deceptive statements did not differ in quality (measured with Criteria-Based Content Analysis [CBCA]) or plausibility. In Study 2, a laboratory experiment, participants in the experimental condition (N = 43) were exposed to an audiotaped truthful and detailed account of an event that was unrelated toinsurance claims (a day at the motor races). The number of words, quality of thestatement (measured with CBCA), and plausibility of the participants’ accounts were compared with participants who were not given a model statement (N = 40). The participants who had listened to the model statement provided longer statements than control participants, truth tellers obtained higher CBCA scores than liars, and only in the model statement condition did truth tellers sound more plausible than liars. Providing participants with a model statement is thus an innovative and successful tool to elicit cues to deception. Providing such a model has the potential to enhance performance in insurance call interviews, and, as we argue, in many other interview settings.

AB - Deception research regarding insurance claims is rare but relevant given the financial loss in terms of fraud. In Study 1, a field study in a large multinational insurance fraud detection company, truth telling mock claimants (N = 19) and lying mock claimants (N = 21) were interviewed by insurance company telephone operators. These operators classified correctly only 50% of these truthful and lying claimants, but their task was particularly challenging: Claimants said little, and truthful and deceptive statements did not differ in quality (measured with Criteria-Based Content Analysis [CBCA]) or plausibility. In Study 2, a laboratory experiment, participants in the experimental condition (N = 43) were exposed to an audiotaped truthful and detailed account of an event that was unrelated toinsurance claims (a day at the motor races). The number of words, quality of thestatement (measured with CBCA), and plausibility of the participants’ accounts were compared with participants who were not given a model statement (N = 40). The participants who had listened to the model statement provided longer statements than control participants, truth tellers obtained higher CBCA scores than liars, and only in the model statement condition did truth tellers sound more plausible than liars. Providing participants with a model statement is thus an innovative and successful tool to elicit cues to deception. Providing such a model has the potential to enhance performance in insurance call interviews, and, as we argue, in many other interview settings.

U2 - 10.1111/lcrp.12017

DO - 10.1111/lcrp.12017

M3 - Journal article

VL - 20

SP - 129

EP - 146

JO - Legal and Criminological Psychology

JF - Legal and Criminological Psychology

SN - 1355-3259

IS - 1

ER -