Home > Research > Publications & Outputs > Learning Verb Syntax via Listening: New Evidenc...

Text available via DOI:

View graph of relations

Learning Verb Syntax via Listening: New Evidence From 22-Month-Olds

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Published

Standard

Learning Verb Syntax via Listening: New Evidence From 22-Month-Olds. / Messenger, Katherine; Yuan, Sylvia; Fisher, Cynthia.
In: Language Learning and Development, Vol. 11, No. 4, 13.12.2014, p. 356-368.

Research output: Contribution to Journal/MagazineJournal articlepeer-review

Harvard

Messenger, K, Yuan, S & Fisher, C 2014, 'Learning Verb Syntax via Listening: New Evidence From 22-Month-Olds', Language Learning and Development, vol. 11, no. 4, pp. 356-368. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2014.978331

APA

Messenger, K., Yuan, S., & Fisher, C. (2014). Learning Verb Syntax via Listening: New Evidence From 22-Month-Olds. Language Learning and Development, 11(4), 356-368. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2014.978331

Vancouver

Messenger K, Yuan S, Fisher C. Learning Verb Syntax via Listening: New Evidence From 22-Month-Olds. Language Learning and Development. 2014 Dec 13;11(4):356-368. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2014.978331

Author

Messenger, Katherine ; Yuan, Sylvia ; Fisher, Cynthia. / Learning Verb Syntax via Listening: New Evidence From 22-Month-Olds. In: Language Learning and Development. 2014 ; Vol. 11, No. 4. pp. 356-368.

Bibtex

@article{92fc080b5a7540328f89055bb230c23a,
title = "Learning Verb Syntax via Listening: New Evidence From 22-Month-Olds",
abstract = "Children recruit verb syntax to guide verb interpretation. We asked whether 22-month-olds spontaneously encode information about a particular novel verb{\textquoteright}s syntactic properties through listening to sentences, retain this information in long-term memory over a filled delay, and retrieve it to guide interpretation upon hearing the same novel verb again. Children watched dialogues in which interlocutors discussed unseen events using a novel verb in transitive (e.g., “Anna blicked the baby”) or intransitive sentences (“Anna blicked”). Children later heard the verb in isolation (“Find blicking!”) while viewing a two-participant causal action and a one-participant action event. Children who had heard transitive dialogues looked longer at the two-participant event than did those who heard intransitive dialogues. This effect disappeared if children heard a different novel verb at test (“Find kradding!”). These findings implicate a role for distributional learning in early verb learning: syntactic-combinatorial information about otherwise unknown words may pervade the toddler{\textquoteright}s lexicon, guiding later word interpretation.",
author = "Katherine Messenger and Sylvia Yuan and Cynthia Fisher",
year = "2014",
month = dec,
day = "13",
doi = "10.1080/15475441.2014.978331",
language = "English",
volume = "11",
pages = "356--368",
journal = "Language Learning and Development",
issn = "1547-5441",
publisher = "Psychology Press Ltd",
number = "4",

}

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - Learning Verb Syntax via Listening: New Evidence From 22-Month-Olds

AU - Messenger, Katherine

AU - Yuan, Sylvia

AU - Fisher, Cynthia

PY - 2014/12/13

Y1 - 2014/12/13

N2 - Children recruit verb syntax to guide verb interpretation. We asked whether 22-month-olds spontaneously encode information about a particular novel verb’s syntactic properties through listening to sentences, retain this information in long-term memory over a filled delay, and retrieve it to guide interpretation upon hearing the same novel verb again. Children watched dialogues in which interlocutors discussed unseen events using a novel verb in transitive (e.g., “Anna blicked the baby”) or intransitive sentences (“Anna blicked”). Children later heard the verb in isolation (“Find blicking!”) while viewing a two-participant causal action and a one-participant action event. Children who had heard transitive dialogues looked longer at the two-participant event than did those who heard intransitive dialogues. This effect disappeared if children heard a different novel verb at test (“Find kradding!”). These findings implicate a role for distributional learning in early verb learning: syntactic-combinatorial information about otherwise unknown words may pervade the toddler’s lexicon, guiding later word interpretation.

AB - Children recruit verb syntax to guide verb interpretation. We asked whether 22-month-olds spontaneously encode information about a particular novel verb’s syntactic properties through listening to sentences, retain this information in long-term memory over a filled delay, and retrieve it to guide interpretation upon hearing the same novel verb again. Children watched dialogues in which interlocutors discussed unseen events using a novel verb in transitive (e.g., “Anna blicked the baby”) or intransitive sentences (“Anna blicked”). Children later heard the verb in isolation (“Find blicking!”) while viewing a two-participant causal action and a one-participant action event. Children who had heard transitive dialogues looked longer at the two-participant event than did those who heard intransitive dialogues. This effect disappeared if children heard a different novel verb at test (“Find kradding!”). These findings implicate a role for distributional learning in early verb learning: syntactic-combinatorial information about otherwise unknown words may pervade the toddler’s lexicon, guiding later word interpretation.

U2 - 10.1080/15475441.2014.978331

DO - 10.1080/15475441.2014.978331

M3 - Journal article

VL - 11

SP - 356

EP - 368

JO - Language Learning and Development

JF - Language Learning and Development

SN - 1547-5441

IS - 4

ER -